Abstract
AbstractThe Productive Failure (PF) approach prompts students to attempt to solve a problem prior to instruction – at which point they typically fail. Yet, research on PF shows that students who are involved in problem solving prior to instruction gain more conceptual knowledge from the subsequent instruction compared to students who receive the instruction first. So far, there is no conclusive evidence, however, that the beneficial effects of PF are explained by the attempt to generate one’s own solutions prior to instruction. The literature on example-based learning suggests that observing someone else engaging in problem-solving attempts may be an equally effective means to prepare students for instruction. In an experimental study, we compared a PF condition, in which students were actively engaged in problem solving prior to instruction, to two example conditions, in which students either observed the complete problem-solving-and-failing process of another student engaging in PF or looked at the outcome of this process (i.e., another student’s failed solution attempts). Rather than worked examples of the correct solution procedure, the students observed examples of failed solution attempts. We found that students’ own problem solving was not superior to the two example conditions. In fact, students who observed the complete PF process even outperformed students who engaged in PF themselves. Additional analyses revealed that the students’ prior knowledge moderated this effect: While students who observed the complete PF process were able to take advantage of their prior knowledge to gain more conceptual knowledge from the subsequent instruction, prior knowledge did not affect students’ post-test performance in the PF condition.
Publisher
Springer Science and Business Media LLC
Subject
Developmental and Educational Psychology,Education
Reference35 articles.
1. Adams, D. M., McLaren, B. M., Durkin, K., Mayer, R. E., Rittle-Johnson, B., Isotani, S., & van Velsen, M. (2014). Using erroneous examples to improve mathematics learning with a web-based tutoring system. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 401–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.053.
2. Ashman, G., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2019). Problem-solving or explicit instruction: Which should go first when element interactivity is high? Educational Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09500-5.
3. Brand, C., Hartmann, C., & Rummel, N. (2018, June 23–27). Exploring relevant problem-solving processes in learning from productive failure. In J. Kay, R. Luckin (Eds), Rethinking learning in the digital age: Making the learning sciences count, 13th International Conference of the Learning Sciences (ICLS) (pp. 1125–1128), Vol. 2. London, UK: International Society of the Learning Sciences. Retrieved December 14, 2020, from https://repository.isls.org/handle/1/576.
4. Darabi, A., Arrington, T. L., & Sayilir, E. (2018). Learning from failure: A meta-analysis of empirical studies. Educational Technology Research and Development, 66(5), 1101–1118. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-018-9579-9.
5. Durkin, K., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to support learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 206–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.001.
Cited by
11 articles.
订阅此论文施引文献
订阅此论文施引文献,注册后可以免费订阅5篇论文的施引文献,订阅后可以查看论文全部施引文献