1. We will use the term tempo when beats per time unit are measured and speed for the perceptive evaluation of this tempo throughout the paper to take into account the distinction between the terms (e.g., see Elowsson & Friberg, 2015).
2. We did not calculate Cronbach's as with stimuli as items, which seems to be done in several previous studies, and which would have given much higher values (>.9) due to the large number of items (N = 60). We think that such a procedure would violate the basic assumptions underlying Cronbach's α derivation, i.e., items are measuring the same construct. This can also be seen by the fact that for N = 60 items, mean correlations of small as r = .14 result in Cronbach's α larger than .90.
3. Ahlbäck, S. (2004). Melody beyond notes: A study of melody cognition (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Göteborg, Sweden. Retrieved from: http://www.uddatoner.com/mer/MBN_ladda_ner/files/MelodyBeyondNotes.pdf
4. Alluri, V., & Toiviainen, P. (2010). Exploring perceptual and acoustical correlates of polyphonic timbre. Music Perception, 27, 223–241. DOI: 10.1525/Mp.2009.27.3.223
5. Alluri, V., Toiviainen, P., Jaaskelainen, I. P., Glerean, E., Sams, M., & Brattico, E. (2012). Large-scale brain networks emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm. Neuroimage, 59, 3677–3689. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.019