Gender Dysphoria and Sexual Euphoria: A Bayesian Perspective on the Influence of Gender-Affirming Hormone Therapy on Sexual Arousal
-
Published:2024-01-12
Issue:5
Volume:53
Page:1859-1871
-
ISSN:0004-0002
-
Container-title:Archives of Sexual Behavior
-
language:en
-
Short-container-title:Arch Sex Behav
Author:
Klöbl ManfredORCID, Reed Murray Bruce, Handschuh Patricia, Kaufmann Ulrike, Konadu Melisande Elisabeth, Ritter Vera, Spurny-Dworak Benjamin, Kranz Georg S., Lanzenberger RupertORCID, Spies Marie
Abstract
AbstractSelf-reported sexual orientation of transgender individuals occasionally changes over transition. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we tested the hypothesis that neural and behavioral patterns of sexual arousal in transgender individuals would shift from the assigned to the experienced gender (e.g., trans women’s responses becoming more dissimilar to those of cis men and more similar to those of cis women). To this aim, trans women (N = 12) and trans men (N = 20) as well as cisgender women (N = 24) and cisgender men (N = 14) rated visual stimuli showing male–female, female–female or male–male intercourse for sexual arousal before and after four months of gender-affirming hormone therapy. A Bayesian framework allowed us to incorporate previous behavioral findings. The hypothesized changes could indeed be observed in the behavioral responses with the strongest results for trans men and female–female scenes. Activation of the ventral striatum supported our hypothesis only for female–female scenes in trans women. The respective application or depletion of androgens in trans men and trans women might partly explain this observation. The prominent role of female–female stimuli might be based on the differential responses they elicit in cis women and men or, in theory, the controversial concept of autogynephilia. We show that correlates of sexual arousal in transgender individuals might change in the direction of the experienced gender. Future investigations should elucidate the mechanistic role of sex hormones and the cause of the differential neural and behavioral findings.The study was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT02715232), March 22, 2016.
Funder
Austrian Science Fund Brain and Behavior Research Foundation Bundesministerium für Wissenschaft, Forschung und Wirtschaft Österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften Medical University of Vienna
Publisher
Springer Science and Business Media LLC
Reference66 articles.
1. Altinay, M., & Anand, A. (2020). Neuroimaging gender dysphoria: A novel psychobiological model. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 14(4), 1281–1297. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00121-8 2. Apostolou, M., Frantzides, N., & Pavlidou, A. (2014). Men competing, men watching: Exploring watching-pattern contingencies in sports. International Journal of Sport Communication, 7(4), 462–476. https://doi.org/10.1123/ijsc.2014-0033 3. Arnow, B. A., Desmond, J. E., Banner, L. L., Glover, G. H., Solomon, A., Polan, M. L., Lue, T. F., & Atlas, S. W. (2002). Brain activation and sexual arousal in healthy, heterosexual males. Brain, 125(5), 1014–1023. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf108 4. Arnow, B. A., Millheiser, L., Garrett, A., Polan, M. L., Glover, G. H., Hill, K. R., Lightbody, A., Watson, C., Banner, L., Smart, T., & Buchanan, T. (2009). Women with hypoactive sexual desire disorder compared to normal females: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience, 158(2), 484–502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.09.044 5. Auer, M. K., Fuss, J., Höhne, N., Stalla, G. K., & Sievers, C. (2014). Transgender transitioning and change of self-reported sexual orientation. PLoS ONE, 9(10), e110016. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110016
|
|