Visual Perception of 3D Space and Shape in Time - Part I: 2D Space Perception by 2D Linear Translation
Author:
Afifa UmaimaORCID, Carmona JavierORCID, Dinh AmyORCID, Espino DiegoORCID, McCarthy TrevorORCID, Ta Brian, Wilson PatrickORCID, Asdell BenjaminORCID, Baik JinwooORCID, Biju ArchanaORCID, Chung SoniaORCID, Dao ChristopherORCID, Diamond MarkORCID, Doust SabaORCID, East AngelaORCID, Espino Diego, Fleiszig-Evans KaileyORCID, Franco AdrianORCID, Garibay-Gutierrez AnthonyORCID, Guha AparajeetaORCID, Gunturu RoshanORCID, Handley LukeORCID, Honore ChristinaORCID, Kannan AbinavORCID, Khoo JaredORCID, Khosla MiraORCID, Kittur ChandanORCID, Kwon AlexandraORCID, Lee JessicaORCID, Lwe NicholasORCID, Mayer MylanORCID, Mills ElizabethORCID, Pineda DelilahORCID, Pourebrahim Pasha, Rajacich Jacob, Rizvi ShanORCID, Rosales LilianaORCID, Schummer LeonardORCID, Sefkow ConorORCID, Stangel AlexanderORCID, Ta Cindy, Ta IvyORCID, Tong NatalieORCID, Tsujimoto KyleORCID, Vu AlyssaORCID, Wang Henry, Yares AmandaORCID, Yamaguchi NatsukoORCID, Yoon Ki WoongORCID, Yu ShuyiORCID, Blaisdell Aaron P.ORCID, Arisaka KatsushiORCID
Abstract
AbstractVisual perception plays a critical role in navigating space and extracting useful semantic information crucial to survival. To identify distant landmarks, we constantly shift gaze vectors through saccades, while still maintaining the visual perception of stable allocentric space. How can we sustain stable allocentric space so effortlessly? To solve this question, we have developed a new concept of NHT (Neural Holography Tomography). This model states that retinotopy is invisible (not available to consciousness) and must be converted to a time code by traveling alpha brainwaves to perceive objects consciously. According to this framework, if identical alpha phases are continually assigned to a landmark, we perceive its exact and consistent allocentric location.To test this hypothesis, we designed reaction time (RT) experiments to observe evidence of the predicted space-to-time conversion. Various visual stimuli were generated at a wide range of eccentricities either on a large TV (up to 40°) or by LED strips on a hemispherical dome (up to 60°). Participants were instructed to report the observed patterns promptly under either covert (no eye movement) or overt (with eye movement) conditions. As predicted, stimuli presented at the center of fixation always produced the fastest RTs. The additional RT delay was precisely proportional to the eccentricity of the peripheral stimulus presentation. Furthermore, both covert and overt attention protocols created the same RT delays, and trajectories of saccadic eye motions were in parallel to the overt RT vs. eccentricity. These findings strongly support our NHT model, in which the observed RT-eccentricity dependence is indicative of the spatiotemporal conversion required for maintaining a stable allocentric frame of reference. That is, we perceive space by time.
Publisher
Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory
Reference24 articles.
1. Ando, S. , Kida, N. , & Oda, S. (2016). Practice Effects on Reaction Time for Peripheral and Central Visual Fields: Perceptual and Motor Skills. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2002.95.3.747 2. Arisaka, K. (2022a). Grand Unified Theory of Mind and Brain Part I: Space-Time Approach to Dynamic Connectomes of C. elegans and Human Brains. 3. Arisaka, K. (2022b). Grand Unified Theory of Mind and Brain Part II: Neural Holographic Tomography (NHT) and Holographic Ring Attractor Lattice (HAL). 4. Arisaka, K. , & Blaisdell, A. (2022). Grand Unified Theory of Mind and Brain Part III: Holographic Visual Perception of 3D Space and Shape. 5. The Involvement of the Two Hemispheres in the Reaction Time to Central and Peripheral Visual Stimuli
|
|