Abstract
AbstractRecent discussions in the literature, along with the revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) [2], suggest aetiological commonalities between the highly comorbid Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Addressing this discussion requires studying these disorders together by comparing constructs typical to each of them. In the present study, we investigate global processing, known to be difficult for participants with ASD, and Intra-Subject Variability (ISV), known to be consistently increased in participants with ADHD, in groups, aged 10-13 years, with ADHD (n=25), ASD without comorbid ADHD (ASD-) (n=13) and ASD with ADHD (ASD+) (n=18) in comparison with a typically developing group (n=22). A Copying task, typically requiring global processing and in this case particularly designed using equally complex stimuli to also measure ISV across trials, was selected. Oculomotor measures in this task proved to be particularly sensitive to group differences. While increased ISV was not observed in the present task in participants with ADHD, both ASD groups needed to look longer on the figure to be drawn, indicating that global processing takes longer in ASD. However, the ASD+ group needed to fixate on the figure only between drawing movements, whereas the ASD-group needed to do this throughout the drawing process. The present study provides evidence towards ASD and ADHD being separate, not-overlapping, disorders. Since the pure ASD-group was affected more by central coherence problems than the ASD+ group, it may suggest that neuropsychological constructs interact differently in different clinical groups and sub-groups.
Publisher
Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory
Reference53 articles.
1. Achenbach, T. M. , & Rescorla, L. A. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: an integrated system of multi-informant assessment Burlington, VT: University of Vermont. 2001. Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families, 16–17.
2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed). 2013. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
3. Biscaldi, M. , Rauh, R. , Müller, C. , Irion, L. , Saville, C. W. N. , Schulz, E. , & Klein, C. Identification of neuromotor deficits common to autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and imitation deficits specific to autism spectrum disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015 Dec. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-015-0753-x
4. Bölte, S. , & Poustka, F. The broader cognitive phenotype of autism in parents: How specific is the tendency for local processing and executive dysfunction? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01603.x
5. Bölte, S. , & Poustka, F. Skala zur erfassung sozialer reaktivität—Dimensionale autismus-diagnostik. In J. N. Constantino & C. P. Gruber (Eds.), German version of the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). 2007. Bern, Switzerland: Huber