The effects of shared, depression-specific, and anxiety-specific internalizing symptoms on negative and neutral episodic memories following post-learning sleep
-
Published:2024-08-13
Issue:
Volume:
Page:
-
ISSN:1530-7026
-
Container-title:Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
-
language:en
-
Short-container-title:Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
Author:
Niu XinranORCID, Utayde Mia F., Sanders Kristin E. G., Cunningham Tony J., Zhang Guangjian, Kensinger Elizabeth A., Payne Jessica D.
Abstract
AbstractEmotional memory bias is a common characteristic of internalizing symptomatology and is enhanced during sleep. The current study employs bifactor S-1 modeling to disentangle depression-specific anhedonia, anxiety-specific anxious arousal, and the common internalizing factor, general distress, and test whether these internalizing symptoms interact with sleep to influence memory for emotional and neutral information. Healthy adults (N = 281) encoded scenes featuring either negative objects (e.g., a vicious looking snake) or neutral objects (e.g., a chipmunk) placed on neutral backgrounds (e.g., an outdoor scene). After a 12-hour period of daytime wakefulness (n = 140) or nocturnal sleep (n = 141), participants judged whether objects and backgrounds were the same, similar, or new compared with what they viewed during encoding. Participants also completed the mini version of the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire. Higher anxious arousal predicted worse memory across all stimuli features, but only after a day spent being awake—not following a night of sleep. No significant effects were found for general distress and anhedonia in either the sleep or wake condition. In this study, internalizing symptoms were not associated with enhanced emotional memory. Instead, memory performance specifically in individuals with higher anxious arousal was impaired overall, regardless of emotional valence, but this was only the case when the retention interval spanned wakefulness (i.e., not when it spanned sleep). This suggests that sleep may confer a protective effect on general memory impairments associated with anxiety.
Funder
National Science Foundation
Publisher
Springer Science and Business Media LLC
Reference144 articles.
1. Adam, E. K., Quinn, M. E., Tavernier, R., McQuillan, M. T., Dahlke, K. A., & Gilbert, K. E. (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 25–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018 2. An, M., Kusurkar, R. A., Li, L., Xiao, Y., Zheng, C., Hu, J., & Chen, M. (2017). Measurement invariance of the strength of motivation for medical school: A multi-group confirmatory factor analysis. BMC Medical Education, 17(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-017-0958-4 3. Anderson, E. R., & Hope, D. A. (2008). A review of the tripartite model for understanding the link between anxiety and depression in youth. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(2), 275–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.05.004 4. Ashton, J. E., Harrington, M. O., Guttesen, A. Á. V., Smith, A. K., & Cairney, S. A. (2019). Sleep preserves physiological arousal in emotional memory. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 5966. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-42478-2 5. Baran, B., Pace-Schott, E. F., Ericson, C., & Spencer, R. M. C. (2012). Processing of emotional reactivity and emotional memory over sleep. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 32(3), 1035–1042. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-11.2012
|
|