1. For the benefit of musicians and nonmusicians who cannot read music notation, we will emphasize note names rather than written music in our discussion. We trust that musicians who can read music notation are likely to already be familiar with these concepts, and so written music would be superfluous.
2. We thank David Temperley for pointing this out to us.
3. We were ambivalent as to whether to include Locrian in the planned comparison, because it differs from the other modes in not having a perfect 5th (G in the present stimuli), and this is presumed to make it particularly unpleasant to Western listeners. Temperley and Tan (2013) noted that it is rarely encountered in Western music, and they did not include it in their experiment. However, as is evident from Table 2, the means for Locrian mode were similar to the other minor modes. Excluding the Locrian makes little difference to the planned comparisons: t(31) = 4.34, p < .0005, for mood; and t(31) = 2.34, p =.03, for tension.
4. Biamonte, N. (2010). Triadic modal and pentatonic patterns in rock music. Music Theory Spectrum, 32, 95–110.
5. Cooke, D. (1959). The language of music. London, UK: Oxford University Press.