Spelling in Finnish: the case of the double consonant
-
Published:2021-11-12
Issue:
Volume:
Page:
-
ISSN:0922-4777
-
Container-title:Reading and Writing
-
language:en
-
Short-container-title:Read Writ
Author:
Marinus EvaORCID, Torppa Minna, Hautala Jarkko, Aro Mikko
Abstract
AbstractBecause of its regularity, it is relatively easy to learn to read and spell in Finnish. However, a specific hurdle in spelling acquisition seems to be the doubling of consonant letters. In this study on consonant letter doubling spelling in Finnish children (91 Grade 1 and 191 Grade 2 children), we asked two questions. First, are items with double consonant letters (e.g., “kissa” [ˈkisːɑ] ‘cat’) indeed harder to spell than single consonant items (e.g., “kisa” [ˈkisɑ] ‘contest’)? Second, is consonant doubling harder for stop consonants (e.g., “takki” [ˈtɑkːi] ‘coat’) than for continuant consonants (e.g., “kissa” [ˈkisːɑ] ‘cat’)? We found that Finnish children made more errors on items with double consonant letters than on items with single consonant letters and that this effect was larger for stop than for continuant consonant letters. Exploratory analyses showed that these effects were stronger for younger and poorer spellers. Post hoc analyses of the errors made on double consonant items showed that the children predominantly made nonlexical errors (> 90%). When they did make a lexical error, these errors typically did not map on the type of errors that would be expected from a corpus analysis of the higher-frequency orthographic neighbors. Overall, lexical influences on spelling of Finnish children seem to be minimal and unpredictable. We discuss two potential reasons why it is more difficult to spell items with double consonant letters than with single consonant letters and suggest how these could be investigated in future research.
Funder
Academy of Finland Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research Council PH Schwyz
Publisher
Springer Science and Business Media LLC
Subject
Speech and Hearing,Linguistics and Language,Education,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology
Reference22 articles.
1. Aro, M. (2017). Learning to read Finnish. In L. Verhoeven & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Learning to read across languages and writing systems (pp. 416–436). https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316155752.017 2. Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavsky, M., Onochie-Quintanilla, E., Salas, N., Schöffelová, M., Defior, S., Mikulajová, M., & Seidlová-Málková Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23(6), 678–686. 3. Friedmann, N., & Rahamim, E. (2007). Developmental letter position dyslexia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 1(Pt 2), 201–236. https://doi.org/10.1348/174866407X204227 4. Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Landerl, K., Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Jong, P. F., & d., & Parrila, R. (2020). Reading and spelling development across languages varying in orthographic consistency: Do their paths cross? Child Development, 91(2), e266–e279. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13218 5. Hautala, J., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Lyytinen, H. (2013). The role of letters and syllables in typical and dysfluent reading in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 26(6), 845–864. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9394-3
Cited by
1 articles.
订阅此论文施引文献
订阅此论文施引文献,注册后可以免费订阅5篇论文的施引文献,订阅后可以查看论文全部施引文献
|
|