Differences in motor imagery abilities in active and sedentary individuals: new insights from backward-walking imagination
-
Published:2023-09-29
Issue:
Volume:
Page:
-
ISSN:0340-0727
-
Container-title:Psychological Research
-
language:en
-
Short-container-title:Psychological Research
Author:
Mandolesi LauraORCID, Passarello NoemiORCID, Lucidi FabioORCID
Abstract
AbstractEvidence has shown that imagining a complex action, like backward-walking, helps improve the execution of the gesture. Despite this, studies in sport psychology have provided heterogeneous results on the use of motor imagery (MI) to improve performance. We aimed to fill this gap by analyzing how sport experience influences backward-walking MI processes in a sample of young women (n = 41, mean age = 21 ± 2.2) divided into Active and Sedentary. All participants were allocated to two randomized mental chronometric tasks, in which they had first to imagine and then execute forward-walking (FW) and backward-walking (BW). The Isochrony Efficiency measured the difference between imagination and execution times in both conditions (FW and BW). Moreover, we analyzed the ability to vividly imagine FW and BW within various perspectives in both groups through the Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ-2). Findings showed that active individuals performed better in the BW imagery task when compared to sedentary ones (F1,39 = 4.98; p = 0.03*), while there were no differences between groups in the FW imagery task (F1,39 = .10; p = 0.75). Further, VMIQ-2 had evidenced that the ability to imagine backward is influenced by perspective used. Specifically, the use of internal visual imagery (IVI) led to worse Isochrony Efficiency (t32,25 = 2.16; p = 0.04*), while the use of kinesthetic imagery (KIN) led to better Isochrony Efficiency (t32,25 = − 2.34; p = 0.03*). These results suggest a close relation between motor experience and complex motor imagery processes and open new insights for studying these mental processes.
Funder
Department of Humanities, University of Naples Federico II Università degli Studi di Napoli Federico II
Publisher
Springer Science and Business Media LLC
Subject
Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous),Developmental and Educational Psychology,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology,General Medicine
Reference68 articles.
1. Adams, L., Guz, A., Innes, J. A., & Murphy, K. (1987). The early circulatory and ventilatory response to voluntary and electrically induced exercise in man. The Journal of Physiology, 383, 19–30. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016393 2. Aksentijevic, A., Brandt, K. R., Tsakanikos, E., & Thorpe, M. J. A. (2019). It takes me back: The mnemonic time-travel effect. Cognition, 182, 242–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.007 3. Burianová, H., Marstaller, L., Rich, A. N., Williams, M. A., Savage, G., Ryan, M., & Sowman, P. F. (2020). Motor neuroplasticity: A MEG-fMRI study of motor imagery and execution in healthy ageing. Neuropsychologia, 146, 107539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107539 4. Burianová, H., Sowman, P. F., Marstaller, L., Rich, A. N., Williams, M. A., Savage, G., Al-Janabi, S., de Lissa, P., & Johnson, B. W. (2016). Adaptive motor imagery: A multimodal study of immobilization-induced brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex, 26(3), 1072–1080. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu287 5. Callow, N., & Hardy, L. (2004). The relationship between the use of kinaesthetic imagery and different visual imagery perspectives. Journal Sports Science., 22, 167–177.
Cited by
3 articles.
订阅此论文施引文献
订阅此论文施引文献,注册后可以免费订阅5篇论文的施引文献,订阅后可以查看论文全部施引文献
|
|