Abstract
Dichromatic subjects can name colors accurately, even though they
cannot discriminate among red-green hues (Jameson & Hurvich, 1978). This result is attributed to a normative
language system that dichromatic observers developed by learning subtle
visual cues to compensate for their impoverished color system. The present
study used multidimensional scaling techniques to compare color
categorization spaces of color-vision deficient (CVD) subjects to those of
normal trichromat (NT) subjects, and consensus analysis estimated the
normative effect of language on categorization. Subjects sorted 140
Munsell color samples in three different ways: a free sorting task
(unlimited number of categories), a constrained sorting task (number of
categories limited to eight), and a constrained naming task (limited to
eight basic color terms). CVD color categories were comparable to those of
NT subjects. For both CVD and NT subjects, a common color categorization
space derived from the three tasks was well described by a
three-dimensional model, with the first two dimensions corresponding to
reddish-greenish and yellowish-bluish axes. However, the third axis, which
was associated with an achromatic dimension in NTs, was not identified in
the CVD model. Individual differences multidimensional scaling failed to
reveal group differences in the sorting tasks. In contrast, the personal
color naming spaces of CVD subjects exhibited a relative compression of
the yellowish-bluish dimension that is inconsistent with the typical
deutan-type color spaces derived from more direct measures of perceptual
color judgments. As expected, the highest consensus among CVDs (77%) and
NTs (82%) occurred in the naming task. The categorization behaviors
studied in this experiment seemed to rely more on learning factors, and
may reveal little about CVD perceptual representation of colors.
Publisher
Cambridge University Press (CUP)
Subject
Sensory Systems,Physiology
Reference16 articles.
1. Davidoff, J. , Davies, I. , & Roberson, D. (1999).Color categories in a stone-age tribe.Nature 398,203–204.
2. Romney, A.K. , Moore, C.C. , Batchelder, W.H. , & Hsia, T.L. (2000).Statistical methods for characterizing similarities and differencesbetween semantic structures.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97,518–523.
3. Müller, M. , Cavonius, C.R. , & Mollon, J.D. (1991).Constructing the color space of the deuteranomalousobserver. InColor Vision deficiencies X, eds. Drum, B. , Moreland, J.D. & Serra, A. , pp.377–387.Dordrecht:Kluwer Academic Publishers.
4. Moore, C.C. , Romney, A.K. , & Hsia, T. (2000).Shared cognitive representations of perceptual and semanticstructures of basic colors in Chinese and English.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97,5007–5010.
5. Paramei, G.V. (1996).Color space of normally sighted and color-deficient observersreconstructed from color naming.Psychological Science 7,311–317.
Cited by
35 articles.
订阅此论文施引文献
订阅此论文施引文献,注册后可以免费订阅5篇论文的施引文献,订阅后可以查看论文全部施引文献